In Chapter 5 we describe the propensity of some brain regions to respond to inputs originating in two or more different sensory systems; we refer to this process as polymodal (or multisensory) integration. Experimental data indicate that information from other sensory modalities fine-tunes the accuracy of auditory systems.
Eric Knudsen et al. (1984) performed elegant studies using an especially acute binaural perceiver: the barn owl. When hunting, owls use both arrival differences and intensity differences to accurately localize sounds at night (Peña and Konishi, 2000). Cells of the avian tectum are arranged in a roughly spherical map of space (Knudsen, 1984; Knudsen and Konishi, 1978). In the owl tectum, both auditory and visual spaces are represented, and the maps for the two senses overlap very closely (Knudsen, 1982). In fact, most cells in the owl tectum are polymodal: they respond to both auditory and visual stimuli, and it is thought that this close alignment of auditory and visual maps of space helps guide behavioral responses toward stimuli.
To assess the impact of early experience on the development of these maps, investigators plugged one ear of adult and baby owls. These birds made large errors in localizing sounds, with responses shifted in the direction of the open ear—presumably because the sound was more intense in that ear, which would normally mean that it had come from that side. Adult animals never seemed to adjust to the earplug, but owls younger than 8 weeks at the time of plugging slowly began to compensate.
It turns out that vision is a key mediator in recalibrating auditory localization: the compensation did not occur if the owls were deprived of vision; and if they were fitted with prism glasses that deviated vision by 10 degrees (see Figure 1), the adjustment of auditory localization was matched to this visual error (Knudsen and Knudsen, 1985). Apparently, the induced mismatch between the auditory and visual tectal maps provoked a remapping of auditory space in the baby owls that was no longer possible in the brains of the adult owls. These changes might arise either from structural modifications of growing neural circuits or from modulations of synaptic effectiveness (Knudsen, 1998). The tight integration of auditory and visual perception probably relates to higher-level mechanisms that direct attention to specific locations in the environment (Winkowski and Knudsen, 2006).
Although in owls only juveniles have auditory systems that can be remodeled through experience, the auditory areas of mammals and other birds show plasticity that persists into adulthood. So, for example, if adult monkeys are trained for several months to discriminate sounds in order to receive a food reward, the cortical representation for the training frequencies becomes substantially larger. Control subjects that just passively listen to the same tones do not show this response (Recanzone et al., 1993). Similarly, conditioning a guinea pig to tones of a particular frequency can cause cortical neurons to shift their response to favor that frequency (N. M. Weinberger, 1998). This remodeling can occur very quickly, on the order of just a few minutes (Fritz et al., 2003), reflecting an adaptive ability to continually tune and retune the auditory cortex to detect biologically significant sounds.
Intriguingly, reward may be a potent contributor to the remodeling of auditory cortex. Each day for 20 days, Bao et al. (2001) presented rats with tones paired with microstimulation of the ventral tegmental area (VTA). VTA stimulation activates the brain’s dopamine-based reward system (see Chapter 4). The investigators subsequently found that cortical representations were greatly increased for tones that occurred shortly before—but not after—VTA stimulation. From an evolutionary perspective, this sort of mechanism is highly adaptive because it specifically shapes the cortex to respond to stimuli that have previously signaled a reward, often in a different sensory modality.
References
Bao, S., Chan, V. T., and Merzenich, M. M. (2001). Cortical remodelling induced by activity of ventral tegmental dopamine neurons. Nature 412: 79–83.
Fritz, J., Shamma, S., Elhilali, M., and Klein, D. (2003). Rapid task-related plasticity of spectrotemporal receptive fields in primary auditory cortex. Nature Neuroscience 6: 1216–1223.
Knudsen, E. I. (1982). Auditory and visual maps of space in the optic tectum of the owl. Journal of Neuroscience 2: 1177–1194.
Knudsen, E. I. (1984). The role of auditory experience in the development and maintenance of sound localization. Trends in Neurosciences 7: 326–330.
Knudsen, E. I. (1998). Capacity for plasticity in the adult owl auditory system expanded by juvenile experience. Science 279: 1531–1533.
Knudsen, E., and Knudsen, P. (1985). Vision guides adjustment of auditory localization in young barn owls. Science 230: 545–548.
Knudsen, E. I., Knudsen, P. F., and Esterly, S. D. (1984). A critical period for the recovery of sound localization accuracy following monaural occlusion in the barn owl. Journal of Neuroscience 4: 1012–1020.
Knudsen, E. I., and Konishi, M. (1978). A neural map of auditory space in the owl. Science 200: 795–797.
Peña, J. L., and Konishi, M. (2000). Cellular mechanisms for resolving phase ambiguity in the owl’s inferior colliculus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA 97: 11787–11792.
Recanzone, G. H., Schreiner, D. E., and Merzenich, M. M. (1993). Plasticity in the frequency representation of primary auditory cortex following discrimination training in adult owl monkeys. Journal of Neuroscience 13: 87–103.
Weinberger, N. M. (1998). Physiological memory in primary auditory cortex: Characteristics and mechanisms. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 70: 226–251.
Winkowski, D. E., and Knudsen, E. I. (2006). Top-down gain control of the auditory space map by gaze control circuitry in the barn owl. Nature 439: 336–339.